Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Anodal tDCS over left parietal cortex expedites recovery from stroke-induced apraxic imitation deficits: a pilot study

Abstract

Background

To date, specific therapeutic approaches to expedite recovery from apraxic deficits after left hemisphere (LH) stroke remain sparse. Thus, in this pilot study we evaluated the effect of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in addition to a standardized motor training on apraxic imitation deficits.

Methods

In a rehabilitation hospital, we assessed apraxic, aphasic, and motor deficits in 30 LH stroke patients before and after a five-day standard programme of motor training combined with either anodal (10 min, 2 mA; n = 14) or sham (10 min, 0 mA, n = 16) tDCS applied in a double-blind fashion over left posterior parietal cortex (PPC). Where appropriate, data were analyzed with either t-test, Fisher’s exact test, or univariate/ repeated measures ANOVA.

Results

Compared to sham tDCS, five sessions of anodal tDCS expedited recovery from apraxic imitation deficits (p < 0.05): Already after 5 days, the anodal tDCS group showed levels of imitation performance that were achieved in the sham tDCS group after 3 months. However, the primary outcome of the study (i.e., anodal tDCS induced improvement of the total apraxia score) failed significance, and there was no significant tDCS effect on apraxia after 3 months. Anodal tDCS improved grip force (of the contra-lesional, i.e., right hand), but had no effect on aphasia.

Conclusions

Data from this pilot study show that repetitive, anodal tDCS over left PPC combined with a standardized motor training expedites recovery from imitation deficits in LH stroke patients with apraxia (relative to sham stimulation). Results suggest that in patients suffering from apraxic imitation deficits a randomized controlled trial (RCT) is warranted that investigates the effects of tDCS applied over PPC in addition to a standardized motor training.

Background

Apraxia is a disorder of motor cognition leading to deficits in imitation, pantomiming, and tool use that cannot solely be explained by paresis or sensory deficits [36]. It is most commonly observed after left hemisphere (LH) damage. Despite the facts that i) apraxic upper limb deficits occur in up to 50% of the patients with a LH stroke [5, 41], and, importantly, ii) they constitute a poor prognostic factor for neurorehabilitation [22], specific therapeutic options for apraxic deficits to date remain sparse [13].

Depending on the direction of the current, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) can modulate cortical excitability [37] and thereby aggravate or ameliorate neurological deficits [16]. To date, however, clinically relevant evidence for the effectiveness of tDCS for improving performance of activities of daily living (ADL) after stroke is of low to moderate quality [14].

Apraxic impairments of the imitation of gestures and pantomiming are frequently observed after LH stroke [23]. Although the pathophysiology of apraxic pantomime deficits is still a matter of debate [32], the pivotal role of the left PPC for (hand) gesture imitation is undisputed [12, 20]. Consistent with such a critical role, single sessions of anodal tDCS over the left posterior parietal cortex (PPC) facilitated gesture processing in healthy subjects [43] and improved the performance of apraxic patients (n = 6) in imitation tasks involving the upper limb [3].

Accordingly, in this pilot study, we assessed whether in combination with a standardized motor training anodal (versus sham) tDCS applied over left PPC expedites recovery from apraxic deficits.

Based on previous work [43], we expected that (only) anodal tDCS over the PPC facilitates recovery from apraxic deficits and in particular apraxic imitation deficits [1], since anodal tDCS leads to increased excitability within the stimulated cortex (here: PPC [37];) and has been shown to temporarily ameliorate deficits in hand gesture imitation [3]. For this reason and due to the reduced resilience of the LH stroke patients, we here focused on anodal (versus sham) tDCS above the PPC. Note that previous studies employing anodal, sham, and cathodal tDCS above different stimulation sites (right/left PPC, right/left primary motor cortex, M1) also found positive effects on gesture processing only for 2 mA anodal tDCS above left PPC [3, 7].

To this end, we included in this study 30 sub-acute LH stroke patients, who at the time of testing were undergoing their regular patient-tailored rehabilitation program including physiotherapy, occupational therapy, speech therapy and neuropsychological therapy in an in-patient setting (rehabilitation hospital).

Methods

Participants

We screened 67 patients undergoing neuro-rehabilitation after a first-ever ischemic LH stroke. Of those, 30 right-handed LH stroke patients (59.1 ± 12.8 years old, 76.5 ± 132.2 days post-stroke at baseline assessment (median: 24.3 days), 5 women) met the inclusion criteria (no previous history of neurological or psychiatric diseases, no alcohol or drug abuse, sufficient knowledge of German, right-handedness, no metal implants in the head/neck region, and no cardiac pacemaker). Premorbid handedness was assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [35]. Note that all patients had a laterality quotient (LQ) ≥ + 80, i.e., were in or above the 5th decile for right-handedness. We used the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, cut-off: < 24 of 30 points, [8]) and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS, cut-off for anxiety: < 8 of 21 points, cut-off for depression: < 8 of 21 points, [47]) to exclude clinically relevant cognitive decline, anxiety, or depression. The modified Rankin Scale (mRS) revealed a mild to moderate disability [38]. Table 1 summarizes the demographic, clinical, and neuropsychological data of the 30 LH stroke patients. Figure 1 depicts the lesion distribution of the current sample of left hemisphere (LH) stroke patients. Note that for four patients, no clinical MRI or CT scan suitable for lesion mapping was available (no consent: n = 1, only initial CT scans available with no demarcated stroke lesion despite persistent deficits: n = 3). Lesion mapping was based on clinical imaging by CT (n = 12) or MRI (n = 14; see also [45]). Using the MRIcron software (http://people.cas.sc.edu/rorden/mricron/index.html), an investigator (JMA or EN) manually transferred the lesions to a T1-weighted template brain (ch2.nii). The lesion mapping was double-checked by the other investigator (EN or JMA); both investigators had to agree on lesion location and extent.

Table 1 Demographic, clinical and neuropsychological data of the LH stroke patient groups undergoing anodal or sham tDCS (applied over left posterior parietal cortex, PPC)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Lesion distribution in the current sample of left hemisphere (LH) stroke patients (n = 26). Note that for four patients no scan suitable for lesion mapping was available. Color shades represent the increasing number of overlapping lesions. Slices with the MNI-z-coordinates from − 7 to 48 are shown

A person who was not involved in the assessment or tDCS stimulation of the LH stroke patients assigned the patients consecutively to either the anodal (n = 14) or sham (n = 16) tDCS groups, which were eventually matched for age and time post-stroke as well as the severity of apraxia, basic motor (dys-)function, and aphasia at baseline assessment (see Table 1). Both stimulation groups comprised LH stroke patients with and without apraxia (anodal tDCS: 9 apraxic and 5 non-apraxic patients, sham tDCS: 11 apraxic and 5 non-apraxic patients).

All patients gave written informed consent before participating in the study. The study was carried out following the ethical principles of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and after obtaining approval by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty in Cologne.

Study description and design

The study comprised a baseline assessment, the stimulation period, and the post-stimulation assessment. Furthermore, 25 (of the 30) LH stroke patients underwent a follow-up assessment (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

The study design consisted of a baseline assessment (3–4 days before first tDCS session), the stimulation period (comprising 5 daily session of either anodal or sham tDCS applied above left posterior parietal cortex, PPC, combined with motor training by three motor tasks, see text), and the post-stimulation assessment (3–4 days after final tDCS session). While all 30 LH stroke patients underwent these three study parts, 25 LH stroke patients performed a follow-up assessment, about 3 months after the final tDCS session. The apraxia and aphasia assessments focused on the Cologne Apraxia Screening (KAS) and the short version of the Aphasia Check-List (ACL-K). Grip force measures reflected the motor impairment. As depicted by the grey box, the baseline assessment, the stimulation period, and the post-stimulation assessment took place while the 30 patients were hospitalized for stroke rehabilitation undergoing a patient-tailored, individualized motor (i.e., physiotherapy and occupational therapy) and cognitive (i.e., speech and neuropsychological therapy) rehabilitation program

To extend previous findings of immediate effects of a single tDCS session on apraxic deficits in neurological patients [3], the primary outcome parameter was the change (post vs. pre-intervention) of the Cologne Apraxia Screening (KAS) total score after repetitive anodal (versus sham) tDCS applied over left PPC, at least 3 days after the last tDCS session. Taking into account the pivotal role of the left PPC in gesture imitation, the secondary outcome parameter was the change of the KAS imitation score at the post-stimulation assessment (compared to the baseline assessment).

The baseline assessment (taking place three or 4 days before the first tDCS session) focused on apraxic deficits, aphasia, and basic motor functions.

Apraxic deficits of the ipsilesional left hand were assessed with the Cologne Apraxia Screening (KAS, [44]). The KAS comprises two types of tasks (pantomime, imitation), each performed with two effectors (bucco-facial gestures, limb gestures). This factorial structure results in the following four KAS subtests (consisting of five items each): 1. pantomime of object use involving bucco-facial movements; 2. pantomime of object use involving limb movements; 3. imitation of bucco-facial gestures; 4. imitation of limb gestures. The test material consists of photos that either depict the object/tool or a woman showing the gesture, minimizing the influence of concurrent aphasic deficits [28, 45]. In the pantomime subtests, one or two points (depending on the complexity) are awarded for predefined features of the pantomime. For example, for the item “pantomiming the use of a toothbrush” the following movement features are scored with one point each: (i) the hand is almost closed to a fist, (ii) the hand is held laterally in front of the mouth, (iii) the mouth is slightly opened, and the teeth are shown, and (iv) circling/ pushing movements of the hand. When a given movement feature is absent, no point is given. For the evaluation of the imitation subtests, four points are given for the correct imitation on the first trial. If the first imitation trial fails, the stimulus photo is shown for a second time. Two points are given for a successful second trial. If the patient also fails on the second trial, no point is given. For each KAS item, the patient can maximally achieve four points. Therefore, a maximum score of 20 points can be achieved in each of the four subtests, and the KAS total score can be maximally 80 points. Based on the normative data of the KAS [13], a psychometric analysis revealed that a patient with a score of 76 or less is considered apraxic.

Furthermore, we examined the actual object use of the ipsilesional left hand with five single objects (hammer, toothbrush, pair of scissors, toy gun, and pencil eraser) and two multiple-object tasks (key & padlock, matchbox & candle [11];). However, in the current sample of stroke patients, object use deficits were rare (n = 2, see also [28, 29]). Note, that the two patients with impaired object use also performed below cut-off in the KAS and were thus classified as apraxic.

We assessed patients for aphasia using the short version of the Aphasia Check-List (ACL-K, [26]). Testing of the motor function of the affected, contralesional right hand comprised an assessment of the degree of paresis (paresis scale of the Medical Research Council (MRC), [34]) and the strength of grip force (using a vigorimeter of the company KLS martin). To account for pre-existing individual differences in muscle strength, we normalized the grip force of the contralesional right hand by the grip force of the ipsilesional left hand.

The stimulation period comprised five tDCS sessions on five consecutive days (Monday to Friday of the week following the baseline assessment; in two patients only four sessions could be performed (Tuesday to Friday) due to a public holiday). We placed the active electrode (35cm2) over the left PPC (i.e., position P3 of the 10/20 EEG-system using an EEG-cap to facilitate positioning). We fixed the return electrode (45cm2) in the right (contralesional) supraorbital position [3]. During (active or sham) stimulation, patients were told not to talk or read and they were free to keep their eyes open or closed. tDCS was applied following current safety guidelines [33], using a constant current of 2 mA for 10 min for anodal tDCS, which resulted in a calculated current density of approximately 0.057 mA/cm2. According to the study mode of the DC-Stimulator (neuroConn GmbH, Ilmenau, Germany), a short stimulation of 2 mA was applied for 20s between the fade-in and fade-out periods, which were identical to the anodal stimulation and lasted 6 s each. This procedure prevented any effective modulation of cortical excitability by tDCS [17] and ensured blinding of the patients, since any putative sensations associated with anodal tDCS (e.g., itching, tingling) would also occur for sham tDCS [19]. The investigator, who applied tDCS (JMA), was blinded for the type of tDCS (anodal or sham) with the help of the NeuroConn study mode, in which an individual code triggered either anodal or sham stimulation of a given patient by the pre-programmed tDCS study device.

To promote motor plasticity, anodal (or sham) tDCS was combined with a standardized motor training, which the patients performed on each day of the stimulation period before and after tDCS. This standardized motor training compromised the following three complex motor tasks: (i) the Jebsen Taylor Hand Function Test (JTHFT, [25]): The JHFT simulates activities of daily living (ADLs) performed with the ipsilesional left hand. It comprises a time-dependent functional assessment of seven subtests (copying a sentence, turning cards over, picking up small objects and placing them in a can, simulated feeding, stacking checkers, and moving large (light and heavy) objects from one location to another); (ii) the imitation test by De Renzi and colleagues [10]: The De Renzi test comprises 24 (12 meaningful, 12 meaningless) intransitive gestures, which have to be imitated (up to three times) with the ipsilesional left hand, and (iii) the Action Research Arm Test (ARAT, [46]): The ARAT comprises a wide range of upper-extremity functions of both hands/arms including grasping, lifting and moving objects of various sizes and performing gross upper-extremity movements.

The post-stimulation assessment was scheduled 3 to 4 days after the last tDCS session. As in the baseline assessment, apraxic deficits (KAS, De Renzi object use test), aphasia (ACL-K), and basic motor functions (MRC paresis scale, grip force) were examined. Baseline and the post-stimulation assessments were performed by JMA, who was blind to the type of tDCS-stimulation at the time of testing.

The baseline assessment, the stimulation period, and the post-stimulation assessment took place while the patients were hospitalized for stroke rehabilitation undergoing a regular patient-tailored rehabilitation program including physiotherapy, occupational therapy, speech therapy, and neuropsychological therapy.

Participants who agreed to a follow-up assessment (including the tests applied for the baseline and post-stimulation assessments, i.e., KAS, ACL-K, and grip force) were revisited at their homes about 3 months after the last tDCS session (mean: 100 ± 9 days). Follow-up assessments were performed by JMA (n = 4) or a research assistant (n = 21), who were blind to the type of tDCS-stimulation at the time of the follow-up assessment.

All assessments were videotaped to allow an independent performance evaluation by a second rater, which were contrasted with the initial ratings by JMA and objectified by means of the interrater reliability of the KAS measurements. Pearson correlation analysis revealed a high interrater agreement for the KAS total scores at baseline (r = 0.945, p < .001), post-stimulation (r = 0.984, p < .001), and follow-up (r = 0.844, p < .001).

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS statistics version 24. Demographic and neuropsychological measures of the two stimulation groups were compared by two-sample t-tests, while their gender distribution was compared with Fisher’s exact test.

Separate univariate ANOVAs analyzed the amount of therapy during the stimulation period with the between-group factors APRAXIA (apraxic, non-apraxic) and STIMULATION (anodal tDCS, sham tDCS).

The medium-term effects of anodal versus sham tDCS applied above the left PPC on apraxia, aphasia, and paresis were operationalized as changes of the KAS total, pantomime and imitation scores (for the ipsilesional hand), the ACL-K scores, and the grip force measures (for the contralesional hand) between the baseline and post-stimulation assessments. These measures were analyzed by separate three-way repeated measure ANOVAs with the within-subject factor TIME (baseline vs. post-stimulation assessments) and the between-subject factors APRAXIA (apraxic, non-apraxic) and STIMULATION (anodal tDCS, sham tDCS).

The long-term stimulation effects on apraxia, aphasia, and paresis were operationalized as group differences of the KAS total, pantomime and imitation scores (for the ipsilesional hand), the ACL-K scores, and the grip force measures (for the contralesional hand) that were present at the follow-up. Also, these measures were analyzed by separate univariate ANOVAs with the between-group factors APRAXIA (apraxic, non-apraxic) and STIMULATION (anodal tDCS, sham tDCS).

Results

Unless stated otherwise, we present all results as mean ± SD.

Behavioral/neuropsychological assessment

The sham (n = 16) and anodal (n = 14) tDCS groups did not differ significantly concerning age, time post-stroke, and gender distribution as well as the severity of the initial apraxic, aphasic, and motor deficits at baseline (see Table 1 for all contrasted demographic and neuropsychological variables, all p > .1).

During their stay in the rehabilitation hospital (and thus also during the study period), patients received a regular, patient-tailored motor (i.e., physiotherapy and occupational therapy) and cognitive (i.e., speech and neuropsychological therapy) rehabilitation program. Accordingly, the amount of rehabilitation treatment varied among patients. On average, patients received 168.8 (± 63.7) minutes of motor therapy per day and 64.4 (± 46.7) minutes of cognitive therapy per day. For the amount of motor as well as cognitive therapy during the stimulation period, the univariate ANOVAs with the between-group factors APRAXIA (apraxic, non-apraxic) and STIMULATION (anodal tDCS, sham tDCS) revealed no significant effects (all p > .1), including the interaction APRAXIA x STIMULATION. Thus, the observed differential tDCS-effects (see below) are most likely not caused by differences in the rehabilitation program during the study period.

Medium-term effects of left parietal anodal tDCS on apraxia

Thirty patients with LH stroke entered the 3-way-ANOVAs evaluating the effect of anodal (versus sham) tDCS applied over left PPC (combined with motor training) on the baseline and post-stimulation apraxia scores.

For the KAS total score (maximum of 80 points), the ANOVA with the factors APRAXIA (apraxic vs. non-apraxic, i.e., KAS scores below vs. above cut-off), STIMULATION (anodal vs. sham tDCS), and TIME (baseline vs. post-stimulation) revealed significant main effects of APRAXIA (i.e., apraxic patients scored significantly lower on the KAS than patients without apraxia; F (1,26) = 6.6, p < .05) and TIME (i.e., from baseline to post-stimulation, stroke patients overall improved in their KAS total scores; F (1,26) = 4.9, p < .05; Fig. 3a). Furthermore, there was a significant interaction APRAXIA by TIME (F (1,26) = 6.7, p < .05), indicating that the KAS total scores of apraxic patients (KAS total score at baseline: 66.7 ± 12.1, KAS total score at post-stimulation: 71.6 ± 9.3; t (19) = − 3.2, p < .01) improved significantly more from baseline to post-stimulation than those of the non-apraxic patients (KAS total score at baseline: 78.1 ± 0.8, KAS total score at post-stimulation: 77.7 ± 2.2; t (9) = 0.8, p > 0.4; Fig. 3a). However, the 3-way-interaction of APRAXIA by TIME by STIMULATION failed to reach significance (F (1,26) = 1,9, p = .18). Thus, the primary outcome of the study (i.e., anodal tDCS induced improvement of the KAS total score) failed to reach significance.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Graphical illustration of the Cologne Apraxia Screening (KAS) total score (a, upper, left panel), the KAS imitation subscore (b, lower, left panel), the relative grip force of the contra-lesional, right hand (c, upper, right panel), and the scores of the short version of the Aphasia Check-List (ACL-K, d, lower, right panel) in the four patient groups with LH stroke across the three assessments (baseline assessment [T1, n = 30], post-stimulation assessment [T2, n = 30], and follow-up assessment [T3, n = 25]). LH stroke patients with apraxia (squares) undergoing anodal tDCS (green, A+ anodal) or sham tDCS (blue, A+ sham), LH stroke patients without apraxia (triangles) undergoing anodal tDCS (black, A- anodal) or sham tDCS (grey, A- sham). Displayed are the means and the standard error of the mean (SEM). a. Apraxic patients scored significantly lower on the KAS than patients without apraxia (§, significant main effects of APRAXIA F (1,26) = 6.6, p < .05). Furthermore, there was a significant interaction APRAXIA by TIME ($, F (1,26) = 6.7, p < .05), indicating that the KAS total scores of apraxic patients (KAS total score at baseline: 66.7 ± 12.1, KAS total score at post-stimulation: 71.6 ± 9.3) improved significantly more from baseline to post-stimulation than those of the non-apraxic patients (KAS total score at baseline: 78.1 ± 8.8, KAS total score at post-stimulation: 77.7 ± 2.2; t (28) = − 2.4, p < .05). b. For the scores of the imitation subtests of the KAS (KAS imi), the asterix (*) indicates the significant 3-way-interaction APRAXIA by TIME by STIMULATION (F (1,26) = 4.6, p < .05): The KAS imitation scores (maximum of 40 points) of apraxic patients who underwent anodal tDCS (green squares) improved significantly more from baseline to post-stimulation (from 31.6 ± 3.3 to 37.1 ± 2.3 points) than those of the apraxic patients undergoing sham tDCS (blue squares, from 32.4 ± 10.6 to 32.4 ± 7.9 points; t (18) = − 2.8, p < .05), while there was no significant modulation by stimulation nor relevant changes with time for the non-apraxic patients (non-apraxic, anodal [black triangles]: 39.6 ± 0.9 to 39.2 ± 1.8 points; non-apraxic, sham [grey triangles]: 38.8 ± 1.8 to 39.2 ± 1.1 points; t (8) = 0.65, p = 0.535). c. Independent of apraxia, there was a significant interaction TIME by STIMULATION (&, F (1,26) = 4.9, p < .05), indicating that the grip force levels of the contralesional, right hand significantly improved from baseline to post-stimulation in the stroke patients who underwent anodal tDCS, while no relevant changes were observed for the sham group (anodal tDCS group [green and black lines]: from 65.8 ± 37.9% to 73.5 ± 42.1%; sham tDCS group [blue and grey lines]: from 77.9 ± 29.7% to 75.6 ± 29.1%). Rel. GF_r = relative grip force of the contra-lesional right hand (in relation to the ipsi-lesional left hand, in %). d. There were no significant differential effects of tDCS on the ACL-K-scores, but a main effect of APRAXIA (#, F (1,26) = 4.6, p < .05), which indicated more severe aphasic deficits in the apraxic patients (squares, 25.5 ± 11.5) compared to patients without apraxia (triangles, 33.8 ± 6.3)

Importantly, we found a significant 3-way-interaction APRAXIA by TIME by STIMULATION for the scores of the imitation subtests of the KAS (F (1,26) = 4.6, p < .05; Fig. 3b), indicating that the KAS imitation scores (maximum of 40 points) of apraxic patients who underwent anodal tDCS above the left PPC improved significantly more from baseline to post-stimulation (from 31.6 ± 3.3 to 37.1 ± 2.3 points, t (8) = − 3.7, p < .01) than those of the apraxic patients undergoing sham tDCS (from 32.4 ± 10.6 to 32.4 ± 7.9 points, t (10) = 0, p = 1), while there was no significant modulation by stimulation nor relevant changes with time for the non-apraxic patients (non-apraxic, anodal: 39.6 ± 0.9 to 39.2 ± 1.8 points, t (4) = 0.41, p > .7; non-apraxic, sham: 38.8 ± 1.8 to 39.2 ± 1.1 points, t (4) = − 0.54, p > .6; see Fig. 3b). In contrast, for the scores of the pantomime subtest of the KAS, no significant effects were found (p > .05).

Further examination of the differential stimulation-related improvements in imitation performance revealed that apraxic patients, who underwent anodal tDCS, achieved similar KAS imitation scores as the non-apraxic patients already at the post-stimulation assessment (p = .102), while the KAS imitation scores between these groups clearly differed at the time of baseline assessment (t (12) = 5.3, p < .001).

Medium-term effects of left parietal anodal tDCS on aphasia and grip force

To evaluate putative tDCS effects on the aphasic deficits of apraxic and non-apraxic patients and to assess whether improvements in apraxia scores were paralleled by changes in aphasia severity [39], we computed an ANOVA with the factors APRAXIA (apraxic vs. non-apraxic), STIMULATION (anodal vs. sham tDCS), and TIME (baseline vs. post-stimulation assessment) for the ACL-K-scores (Fig. 3d). Except for a main effect of APRAXIA (F (1,26) = 4.6, p < .05), which indicated more severe aphasic deficits in the apraxic patients (25.5 ± 11.5) compared to patients without apraxia (33.8 ± 6.3; cf. [45]), no significant effects were observed (all p > .06, Fig. 3d). Furthermore, we computed a complementary ANOVA for the ACL-K-scores with the factors APHASIA (aphasic vs. non-aphasic), STIMULATION (anodal vs. sham tDCS), and TIME (baseline vs. post-stimulation assessment). This analysis revealed the two significant main effects of TIME (F (1,26) = 9.2, p < .01), indicating an overall improvement of the ACL-K-scores between the two assessments (baseline: 27.2 ± 11.0, post-stimulation: 29.5 ± 10.5), and APHASIA (F (1,26) = 45.7, p < .001), indicating (i.e., by definition) lower ACL-K-Scores in the aphasic (18.3 ± 8.8) than non-aphasic (36.1 ± 1.8) patients. The other main effects and interactions did not reach significance (all p > .08).

To evaluate whether the improvements of apraxia scores (as a marker for stroke-related motor-cognitive deficits assessed with the ipsi-lesional, left hand) are paralleled by changes in basic motor functions after stroke (here: grip force levels of the contra-lateral, right hand), we computed for the grip force measures of the contralesional, right hand (normalized by the grip force of the ipsilesional, left hand to account for pre-existing individual differences in muscle strength) an ANOVA with the factors APRAXIA (apraxic vs. non-apraxic), STIMULATION (anodal vs. sham tDCS), and TIME (baseline vs. post-stimulation assessment). This ANOVA revealed that - irrespective of the presence/absence of apraxia - grip force levels of the contralesional right hand significantly improved from baseline to post-stimulation in the stroke patients who underwent anodal tDCS, while no changes were observed for the sham group (anodal tDCS group: from 65.8 ± 37.9% to 73.5 ± 42.1%, t (13) = − 2.52, p < .05; sham tDCS group: from 77.9 ± 29.7% to 75.6 ± 29.1%, t (15) = 0.95, p > .3; significant interaction TIME by STIMULATION: F (1,26) = 4.9, p < .05, Fig. 3c). This finding indicates a facilitatory effect of anodal tDCS on an essential primary motor function independent of apraxia. All other comparisons did not reveal any significant changes in grip force (p > .3). Note that all apraxia assessments (e.g., KAS) were performed with the ipsilesional, left hand.

An additional ANOVA with the between-subject factors PARESIS (patients with relevant paresis (i.e., MRC grade ≤ 3) vs. patients without relevant paresis (i.e., MRC grade ≥ 4)) and STIMULATION (anodal vs. sham tDCS) and the within-subject factor TIME (baseline vs. post-stimulation assessment) revealed that - by definition - grip force levels of the contralesional, right hand were lower in stroke patients with (11.7 ± 19.9%) versus without (84.4 ± 19.8%) paresis (main effect of PARESIS: F (1,26) = 43.9, p < .001). Again, the other main effects and interactions did not reach significance (all p > .2).

Long-term effects of left parietal anodal tDCS on apraxia, aphasia, and paresis

Twenty-five (of the initial 30) patients with LH stroke were available for the follow-up assessment after 3 months (100 ± 9 days). We entered the scores derived from the comprehensive follow-up assessment into ANOVAs with the between-group factors APRAXIA (apraxic vs. non-apraxic) and STIMULATION (anodal vs. sham tDCS). For all applied tests (including the KAS, ACL-K, and grip force measures), no significant main effects or interactions were observed (all p > .1, see also Fig. 3), indicating that the current LH stroke patients showed comparable test performance at the follow-up assessment independent of the initial presence of apraxic deficits or the applied stimulation protocol (anodal vs. sham tDCS). Thus, there were no significant effects of anodal tDCS on apraxia after 3 months.

Note that the KAS imitation scores at post-stimulation assessment (37.11 ± 2.26) of the apraxic patients, who had received anodal tDCS, were comparable to the KAS imitation scores at the follow-up assessment (37.4 ± 2.67) of the apraxic patients who had received sham tDCS (p = .804, see Fig. 3). In other words, apraxic patients, who underwent anodal tDCS, achieved KAS imitation scores already after about 10 days that were comparable to those achieved by the apraxic patients undergoing sham tDCS after about 100 days.

Discussion

In this double-blinded, sham-controlled tDCS pilot study we observed that in LH stroke patients the recovery from apraxic imitation deficits is significantly expedited by repetitive anodal tDCS (relative to sham tDCS) applied over left posterior parietal cortex (PPC) combined with standardized motor training. In fact, after five consecutive tDCS-sessions, the imitation performance of the initially apraxic LH stroke patients undergoing anodal parietal stimulation recovered so much that it was comparable to that of the non-apraxic patients. However, the primary outcome of the study (i.e., tDCS induced improvement of the KAS total score) failed significance, and there was no significant effect of anodal tDCS on apraxia after 3 months.

The current study confirms but, importantly, extends previous findings of positive single session tDCS effects on apraxic deficits in neurological patients [3] in various ways. Unique features of the current study are the use of a control patient group, double-blinding of tDCS conditions, and follow-up assessments. In contrast to previous tDCS studies (e.g., n = 6 stroke patients in Bolognini et al. [3]), the current pilot study employed 30 patients. Nevertheless, future studies need to include larger sample sizes, since the effect sizes of tDCS interventions in stroke patients are weak to moderate [6, 27]. Importantly, while previous studies revealed only immediate effects directly after tDCS (i.e., minutes after the end of stimulation), the current data show a significant effect on apraxic imitation deficits lasting at least 3 days after the last session of parietal anodal tDCS. Moreover, the current tDCS effects were controlled for unspecific effects by employing a control patient group.

Furthermore, the enhanced recovery of grip force of the contralesional right hand in the LH stroke patients undergoing anodal but not sham tDCS (independent of apraxia) suggests an additional positive modulation of an essential motor function by anodal tDCS applied over left PPC and thereby extends previous findings of studies modulating the primary motor cortex of the affected hemisphere [2, 24, 42]. This novel finding warrants further investigation in future studies.

In contrast to imitation, there was no significant effect of tDCS on pantomime performance. This lack of an effect can be explained by the notion that over and above an involvement of the left parietal cortex many different areas within a distributed left-hemispheric network are critically involved in pantomiming the use of objects [32]. Thus, the current stimulation above the left PPC was able to ameliorate imitation performance due to the essential role of the left PPC in the processing of gestures for imitation [1, 43], but failed to improve pantomiming of object use, since further relevant areas for pantomiming (e.g., ventral stream areas [23] or frontal regions [45]) were outside the stimulated brain region (i.e., left PPC).

Despite relevant aphasic deficits (see Table 1), anodal parietal tDCS improving apraxic deficits did not have concurrent effects on aphasia in the current patient sample. The most parsimonious explanation for this observation is that those brain regions (e.g., frontal cortex [30], motor cortex [31], temporal cortex [18]) that have been targeted by tDCS studies on aphasic deficits after stroke [15] were outside the current stimulation focus. Notably, Bolognini and colleagues also did not find an effect of tDCS delivered to the left PPC on phonemic fluency in their six stroke patients [3].

Concerning the specificity of the tDCS effects on apraxic deficits, note that (anodal and sham) tDCS was combined with a standardized motor training consisting of three complex motor tasks that the patients performed on each day of the stimulation period before and after tDCS. Thus, our data suggest that the combination of anodal tDCS applied over left PPC and a standardized motor training expedited the recovery from apraxic imitation deficits. This finding supports the current notion that the specificity of tDCS in a given domain may result from the task-specific activation of the critical nodes within a domain-specific network (here: praxis network) already activated by anodal tDCS [9, 16].

Limitations

The fact that study participants were not randomly, but rather consecutively assigned to the anodal and sham tDCS groups by a person who was not involved in the assessments or tDCS stimulation of the LH stroke patients is a limitation of the current pilot study. Therefore, aspects that were not captured by the initial clinical or neuropsychological assessments (e.g., level of motivation) could have influenced the results. However, at baseline the two patient groups were matched for essential parameters: age, time post-stroke, severity of apraxia and aphasia as well as basic motor (dys-)function (see Table 1). Nevertheless, forthcoming tDCS studies on stroke recovery should employ randomization procedures. Note that the current study is the first that employed a control group with apraxic LH stroke patients (cf. [3]).

In healthy young participants, recent studies reported compromised blinding of tDCS conditions [21, 40]. Thus, the lack of a systematic assessment of blinding efficacy constitutes a limitation of our study. However, the participants of the current pilot study were LH stroke patients suffering from cognitive (e.g., apraxic, aphasic) deficits of different severity, which most likely have reduced their ability to distinguish real from sham tDCS, as has previously shown in clinical populations [19]. Nevertheless, future clinical tDCS studies should include a systematic assessment of blinding efficacy (e.g., by using questionnaires as in [4]) and employ optimized sham tDCS protocols [17].

Conclusions

Our data show for the first time that repetitive, anodal tDCS over left PPC combined with a standardized motor training expedites recovery from imitation deficits in LH stroke patients with apraxia. However, the primary outcome of the study (i.e., anodal tDCS induced improvement of the total apraxia score) failed significance, and there was no significant tDCS effect on apraxia after 3 months. Nevertheless, based on these findings, a randomized controlled trial with an even larger sample of LH stroke patients suffering from (imitation) apraxia is warranted.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ACL-K:

Aphasia Check-List (short version)

HADS:

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale

KAS:

Cologne Apraxia Screening

LH:

Left hemisphere

LQ:

Laterality Quotient as assessed by the Edinburgh handedness inventory

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

MRC:

Medical Research Council

mRS:

Modified Rankin Scale

PPC:

Posterior parietal cortex

tDCS:

Transcranial direct current stimulation

References

  1. Achilles, E. I. S., Ballweg, C. S., Niessen, E., Kusch, M., Ant, J. M., Fink, G. R., & Weiss, P. H. (2019). Neural correlates of differential finger gesture imitation deficits in left hemisphere stroke. NeuroImage: Clinical, 23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101915.

  2. Boggio, P. S., Nunes, A., Rigonatti, S. P., Nitsche, M. A., Pascual-Leone, A., & Fregni, F. (2007). Repeated sessions of noninvasive brain DC stimulaton is associated with motor function improvement in stroke patients. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 25, 123–129.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Bolognini, N., Convento, S., Banco, E., Mattioli, F., Tesio, L., & Vallar, G. (2015). Improving ideomotor limb apraxia by electrical stimulation of the left posterior parietal cortex. Brain, 138(2), 428–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Brunoni, A. R., Amadera, J., Berbel, B., Volz, M. S., Rizzerio, B. G., & Fregni, F. (2011). A systematic review on reporting and assessment of adverse effects associated with transcranial direct current stimulation. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 14(8), 1133–1145. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145710001690.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Cantagallo, A., Maini, M., & Rumiati, R. I. (2012). The cognitive rehabilitation of limb apraxia in patients with stroke. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 22(3), 473–488. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2012.658317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Chhatbar, P. Y., Ramakrishnan, V., Kautz, S., George, M. S., Adams, R. J., & Feng, W. (2016). Transcranial direct current stimulation post-stroke upper extremity motor recovery studies exhibit a dose-response relationship. Brain Stimulation, 9(1), 16–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2015.09.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Convento, S., Bolognini, N., Fusaro, M., Lollo, F., & Vallar, G. (2014). Neuromodulation of parietal and motor activity affects motor planning and execution. Cortex, 57, 51–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.03.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Creavin, S. T., Wisniewski, S., Noel-Storr, A. H., Trevelyan, C. M., Hampton, T., Rayment, D., et al. (2016). Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the detection of dementia in clinically unevaluated people aged 65 and over in community and primary care populations. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 1, CD011145. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD011145.pub2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Crinion, J. T. (2016). Transcranial direct current stimulation as a novel method for enhancing aphasia treatment effects. European Psychologist, 21(1), 65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. De Renzi, E., Motti, F., & Nichelli, P. (1980). Imitating gestures. A quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. Archives of Neurology, 37, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. De Renzi, E., Pieczuro, A., & Vignolo, L. A. (1968). Ideational apraxia: a quantitative study. Neuropsychologia, 6, 41–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Dovern, A., Fink, G. R., Saliger, J., Karbe, H., Koch, I., & Weiss, P. H. (2011). Apraxia impairs intentional retrieval of incidentally acquired motor knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(22), 8102–8108.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Dovern, A., Fink, G. R., & Weiss, P. H. (2012). Diagnosis and treatment of upper limb apraxia. Journal of Neurology, 259(7), 1269–1283. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-011-6336-y.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Elsner, B., Kugler, J., Pohl, M., & Mehrholz, J. (2016). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) for improving activities of daily living, and physical and cognitive functioning, in people after stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 3, CD009645.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Elsner, B., Kugler, J., Pohl, M., & Mehrholz, J. (2019). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) for improving aphasia in adults with aphasia after stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 5, CD009760. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD009760.pub4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Floel, A. (2014). tDCS-enhanced motor and cognitive function in neurological diseases. Neuroimage, 85(3), 934–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Fonteneau, C., Mondino, M., Arns, M., Baeken, C., Bikson, M., Brunoni, A. R., et al. (2019). Sham tDCS: A hidden source of variability? Reflections for further blinded, controlled trials. Brain Stimulation, 12(3), 668–673. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2018.12.977.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Fridriksson, J., Rorden, C., Elm, J., Sen, S., George, M. S., & Bonilha, L. (2018). Transcranial direct current stimulation vs sham stimulation to treat aphasia after stroke: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Neurology, 75(12), 1470–1476. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.2287.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Gandiga, P. C., Hummel, F., & Cohen, L. G. (2006). Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): A tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117, 845–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Goldenberg, G., & Karnath, H.-O. (2006). The neural basis of imitation is body part specific. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(23), 6282–6287.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Greinacher, R., Buhot, L., Moller, L., & Learmonth, G. (2019). The time course of ineffective sham-blinding during low-intensity (1 mA) transcranial direct current stimulation. Eur J Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14497.

  22. Hanna-Pladdy, B., Heilman, K. M., & Foundas, A. L. (2003). Ecological implications of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 60, 487–490.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Hoeren, M., Kümmerer, D., Bormann, T., Beume, L., Ludwig, V. M., Vry, M.-S., et al. (2014). Neural bases of imitation and pantomime in acute stroke patients: Distinct streams for praxis. Brain, 137(10), 2796–2810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Hummel, F., Celnik, P., Giraux, P., Floel, A., Wu, W.-H., Gerloff, C., & Cohen, L. G. (2005). Effects of non-invasive cortical stimulation on skilled motor function in chronic stroke. Brain, 128, 490–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Jebsen, R. H., Taylor, N., Trieschmann, R. B., Trotter, M. J., & Howard, L. A. (1969). An objective and standardized test of hand function. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 50(6), 311–319.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Kalbe, E., Reinhold, N., Brand, M., Markowitsch, H. J., & Kessler, J. (2005). A new test battery to assess aphasic disturbances and associated cognitive dysfunctions - german normative data on the aphasia check list. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 779–794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Kang, N., Summers, J. J., & Cauraugh, J. H. (2016). Transcranial direct current stimulation facilitates motor learning post-stroke: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 87(4), 345–355. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2015-311242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Kusch, M., Gillessen, S., Saliger, J., Karbe, H., Binder, E., Fink, G. R., et al. (2018). Reduced awareness for apraxic deficits in left hemisphere stroke. Neuropsychology, 32(4), 509–515. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000451.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Kusch, M., Schmidt, C. C., Goden, L., Tscherpel, C., Stahl, J., Saliger, J., et al. (2018). Recovery from apraxic deficits and its neural correlate. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 36(6), 669–678. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-180815.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Marangolo, P., Fiori, V., Calpagnano, M. A., Campana, S., Razzano, C., Caltagirone, C., & Marini, A. (2013). tDCS over the left inferior frontal cortex improves speech production in aphasia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 539. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00539.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Meinzer, M., Darkow, R., Lindenberg, R., & Flöel, A. (2016). Electrical stimulation of the motor cortex enhances treatment outcome in post-stroke aphasia. Brain, 139(4), 1152–1163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Niessen, E., Fink, G. R., & Weiss, P. H. (2014). Apraxia, pantomime and the parietal cortex. Neuroimage Clinical, 5, 42–52.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008. Brain Stimulation, 1, 206–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. O'Brien, M. D. f. t. G. o. B. (2000). Aids to the examination of the peripheral nervous system. London: W. B. Saunders.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Osiurak, F., & Rossetti, Y. (2017). Definition: Limb apraxia. Cortex, 93, 228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.03.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Polania, R., Nitsche, M. A., & Ruff, C. C. (2018). Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nature Neuroscience, 21(2), 174–187. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Rankin, J. (1957). Cerebral vascular accidents in patients over the age of 60. II. Prognosis. Scottish Medical Journal, 2, 200–215.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Smania, N., Aglioti, S., Girardi, F., Tinazzi, M., Fiaschi, A., Cosentino, A., & Corato, E. (2006). Rehabilitation of limb apraxia improves daily life activities in patients with stroke. Neurology, 67, 2050–2052.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Turi, Z., Csifcsak, G., Boayue, N. M., Aslaksen, P., Antal, A., Paulus, W., … Mittner, M. (2019). Blinding is compromised for transcranial direct current stimulation at 1 mA for 20 min in young healthy adults. Eur J Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14403.

  41. Vanbellingen, T., & Bohlhalter, S. (2011). Apraxia in neurorehabilitation: Classification, assessment and treatment. NeuroRehabilitation, 28(2), 91–98. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-2011-0637.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Volz, L. J., Rehme, A. K., Michely, J., Nettekoven, C., Eickhoff, S. B., Fink, G. R., & Grefkes, C. (2016). Shaping early reorganisation of neural networks promotes motor function after stroke. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 2882–2894.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Weiss, P. H., Achilles, E. I. S., Moos, K., Hesse, M. D., Sparing, R., & Fink, G. R. (2013). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of left parietal cortex facilitates gesture processing in healthy subjects. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(49), 19205–19211.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Weiss, P. H., Kalbe, E., Scherer, A., Binder, E., Kessler, J., & Fink, G. R. (2013). Das Kölner Apraxie-Screening (KAS). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Weiss, P. H., Ubben, S. D., Kaesberg, S., Kalbe, E., Kessler, J., Liebig, T., & Fink, G. R. (2016). Where language meets meaningful action: A combined behavior and lesion analysis of aphasia and apraxia. Brain Structure & Function, 221(1), 563–576. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0925-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Yozbatiran, N., Der-Yeghiaian, L., & Cramer, S. C. (2008). A standarized approach to performing the action research arm test. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 22(1), 78–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67(6), 361–370.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all our patients.

Funding

Dr. Fink gratefully acknowledges support from the Marga and Walter Boll foundation. The study was not industry-sponsored.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JMA: study concept and design, acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data, writing of the manuscript. EN: analysis and interpretation of data, writing of the manuscript. EISA: study concept and design. JS: acquisition of data. HK: study supervision. PHW: study concept, design and supervision, analyses and interpretation of data, writing of the manuscript. GRF: study concept and design, interpretation of the data, critical revision of the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the submitted manuscript. The manuscript has not been submitted elsewhere nor published elsewhere in whole or in part, except as an abstract. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gereon R. Fink.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All patients gave written informed consent before participating in the study. The study was carried out following the ethical principles of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and after obtaining approval by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty in Cologne (09–137).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Dr. Fink reports personal fees (for speaking engagements) from Bayer, Lilly, Teva, Boehringer Ingelheim, Sanofi, Desitin, and Glaxo Smith Kline, outside the area of the submitted work.

Dres. Ant, Niessen, Achilles, Saliger, Karbe and Weiss declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ant, J.M., Niessen, E., Achilles, E.I.S. et al. Anodal tDCS over left parietal cortex expedites recovery from stroke-induced apraxic imitation deficits: a pilot study. Neurol. Res. Pract. 1, 38 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s42466-019-0042-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s42466-019-0042-0

Keywords